Running Head: CHILDREN’S KNOWLEDGE OF TEACHING Children’s developing knowledge of and reflection about teaching
نویسندگان
چکیده
Word Count = 122) Fourto 7-year-olds (N=61) defined “teaching” and described what and how others had taught them, and what and how they had taught others. While 4-5-year-olds were often unable to define teaching, 6and 7-year-olds most frequently defined teaching by describing processes that could cause knowledge change. Children who held process-based definitions were more likely to offer examples of what others had taught them, to identify who had taught them, and to describe being taught through direct instruction. They were also better able to describe how they had taught others. We consider the results in light of previous interviews in which children were asked to define learning, and discuss the implications for children’s developing understanding of the connections between knowledge, learning, and teaching. Children’s Knowledge of Teaching 3 Children’s developing knowledge of and reflection about teaching A fundamental aspect of the human experience is the communication of information (Bloom, 2000; Tomasello, 2010). When communication is intended to change the knowledge state of another person, the communicative act can be described as teaching. Some have argued that teaching is a “natural cognitive ability” (Strauss, 2005, p. 368), in that teaching is a universal and basic form of communication that children learn through everyday social interactions rather than though explicit instruction. Even children as young as 3 can engage in acts of teaching (e.g., Ashley & Tomasello, 1998), and school-aged children often explicitly teach others (e.g., Brown & Palincsar, 1989; Flynn, 2010; Rogoff, 1990). Moreover, teaching is related to children’s developing knowledge of their own and others’ mental states — in particular, what children understand about knowledge and beliefs (e.g., Astington, & Pelletier, 1996; Strauss, Ziv, & Stein, 2002; Wellman & Lagattuta, 2004; Ziv & Frye, 2004; Ziv, Solomon, & Frye, 2008). For instance, children’s understanding of their own and others’ knowledge allows them to infer whom to teach and to recognize situations that necessitate teaching because teaching can be motivated by recognizing knowledge gaps between individuals. The more children understand about knowledge, the more they should also recognize how different kinds of actions could be effective to support successful teaching. This line of research largely suggests that children’s developing knowledge of teaching is related to their developing theory of mind – or understanding of others’ mental states generally construed (see Knutsen, Frye & Sobel, 2014, for a review). Children’s understanding of teaching has usually been investigated in two ways: by examining what aspects of others’ behavior children use to identify teaching, and by Children’s Knowledge of Teaching 4 investigating how children actually go about teaching others. As examples of the first, Ziv et al. (2008) showed that 5-year-olds but not 3-year-olds recognized that teaching occurred only when an individual intentionally demonstrated action to another person, and when this person was attending to the action. The situations that they set up examined children’s judgments about whether teaching had occurred, or in other cases, whom a teacher would teach, based on the mental states of the teacher and potential students. As an example of the second, Ashley and Tomasello (1998) introduced pairs of children between 24-42 months with a novel goal-directed task, which they learned collaboratively. Once the pair had mastered the task together, one of the children was paired with a naïve partner, and the authors examined whether and how the more knowledgeable child taught the novice. The youngest children in the study could not engage in such communicative acts, but by 30 months, children communicated with one another about the task, and 42-month-olds engaged in intentional communication through specific, directed actions. These data suggest that even young children begin to integrate their understanding of their own and others’ knowledge into both the judgments they make about teaching and the ways they communicate information in order to teach others. What these studies do not do is examine how children define and reflect on their own experiences of teaching outside of the laboratory. If children’s understanding of teaching is related to their developing knowledge of others’ mental states, then one might expect to see further development past the fourth birthday, as children’s ability to reflect on their own thoughts, pretenses, and emotional responses all develops between the ages of 4-8 (e.g., Eisbach, 2004; Flavell et al., 1995; Lagattuta & Wellman, 2001; Richert & Lillard, 2002). In particular, children’s understanding of what teaching entails, and their metacognitive awareness of what and how they have been taught may develop during this period. Children’s Knowledge of Teaching 5 Although children’s explicit understanding of teaching remains relatively unknown, there have been studies examining children’s developing ability to reflect on their learning. Bemis, Leichtman, and Pillemer (2011) asked 4to 9-year-olds questions whose answers they were likely to know. After giving the correct answer, children were asked how they had learned that answer. Even the youngest children could say how they learned the information, but their ability to do so was limited. Only 25% of 4-5-year-olds stated the source of their knowledge; 7-9-yearolds did so significantly more often, but only 45% of the sample did so. Sobel and Letourneau (2015) conducted a similar investigation, asking 4to 10-year-olds what they thought “learning” meant. After children defined learning, the experimenter asked them to give examples of what they had learned in the past and describe how they had learned in each case. Almost all of the 8-10-year-olds they examined (95%) defined learning as a process involving a source or strategy that would result in a change in knowledge (e.g., “when your teacher tells you something”; “when you practice again and again until you know it”), while only ~42% of the 4-5-year-olds defined learning in that way. Their findings showed a clear linear trajectory for children’s understanding of learning as a process. Most of the younger children did not define learning as a process; instead, they described it based on the types of content that could be learned (e.g., “like reading and math,” ~16%), defined learning circularly (e.g., “learning is when you learn,” ~2%), or did not respond to the question (~40%). Independent of age, the way in which children defined learning related to their ability to reflect on the ways in which they had learned information themselves. Children who defined learning as a process of knowledge acquisition were more likely to describe a source or strategy through which they had learned. Children’s Knowledge of Teaching 6 In the present study, we borrowed this approach to ask children what they thought teaching meant, and then asked them to reflect on situations in which they had been taught and in which they had taught others. Our goal was to describe children’s developing understanding of teaching as a process that causes knowledge change. We also examined the relation between children’s understanding teaching as a means of causing knowledge change and their capacity to reflect on what and how they had been taught and had taught others. The current study focused on 4-7-year-olds for two reasons. First, although we did not present these children with an explicit measure of false belief, the majority of 4-7-year-olds would be expected to pass standard versions of the false belief task (e.g., Wellman, Cross & Watson, 2001), although there might be some age-related change between the younger and older children in our sample. Second, as reported above, not all of the children in Sobel and Letourneau’s sample defined learning as a process, and children showed clear individual differences in their ability to reflect on instances of their own learning. We wished to document whether similar differences existed for teaching and determine the extent to which children’s definitions of teaching related to their ability to reflect on their own experiences involving teaching.
منابع مشابه
Chinese Young Children’s Strategies on Basic Addition Facts
Kindergartens in China offer structured full-day programs for children aged 3-6. Although formal schooling does not commence until age 7, the mathematics program in kindergartens is specifically focused on developing young children’s facility with simple addition and subtraction. This study explored young Chinese children’s strategies for solving basic addition facts as well as their intuitive ...
متن کاملTheory of mind and children’s understanding of teaching and learning during early childhood
How children understand the concepts of teaching and learning is inherently underpinned by their mental state understanding and critical to the successful transition to formal schooling. Knowledge is a private representational mental state; learning is a knowledge change process that can be either intentional or not; and teaching is an intentional attempt to change others’ knowledge state. Theo...
متن کاملRunning Head: GAZE AVERSION AND TRANSIENT KNOWLEDGE Gaze Aversion During Children’s Transient Knowledge and Learning
Looking away from an interlocutor’s face during demanding cognitive activity can help adults and children answer challenging mental arithmetic and verbal-reasoning questions (Glenberg, Schroeder, & Robertson, 1998; Phelps, Doherty-Sneddon, & Warnock, 2006). While such “gaze aversion” (GA) is used far less by 5-year old school children, its use increases dramatically during the first years of pr...
متن کاملVariations in EFL Teachers’ Pedagogical Knowledge Base as a Function of Their Teaching License Status
The study of teachers’ pedagogical knowledge base (PKB) to discover how teachers think and work is attracting increasing attention in ELT. Against this background, the present study aimed at probing the likely variations in EFL teachers’ pedagogical knowledge base as a function of their teaching license status. To this aim, six teachers (two standard-licensed, two alternatively-licensed, and tw...
متن کاملRunning Head: Predictors of Children’s Construction Task
Construction tasks can be linked to achievement in maths and science and form part of school curricula. However, there is little foundation for their use in teaching as there are no apparent methods for assessing difficulty. This empirical research identifies four construction task characteristics that impact on cognition and influence construction task difficulty in children aged 7-8 and 10-11...
متن کاملذخیره در منابع من
با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید
عنوان ژورنال:
دوره شماره
صفحات -
تاریخ انتشار 2015